PEPonline
Professionalization
of Exercise Physiologyonline

An international electronic
journal for exercise physiologists
ISSN 1099-5862

Vol 4 No 6 June 2001

 

The Use of Imagery in Teaching
Tommy Boone, PhD, MPH, FASEP, EPC
Professor and Chair 
Department of Exercise Physiolgoy
College of St. Scholastica
Duluth, MN 55811

MENTAL IMAGE IS one of the oldest, neglected topics in cognitive science.  It is so mis-understood that psychologists have supported and yet denied it, even though it has always been with us.  Few would argue against one’s ability to create a mental image.  Everyone creates images and, seemingly, most everyone disagrees on what is a mental image.  To think about what is an image is to ask a log of questions without obvious answers.  No one feels certain about a topic or comfortable speaking about a topic when there are no answers.  Both neglect and speculation provides little support in the use of mental images that are used to facilitate learning.  Teachers above everyone else should figure out whether there is something to it.  That is, can “we” (meaning, students and teachers) create mental pictures inside the brain?  If so, how do we define a mental picture and, if it can be defined, what type of information can the picture provide us?  The answers to these questions remain a challenge for even the best professional. 

If images exist, can a teacher (or student) be taught to recall the image?  Is the recall based on memorization?  If it can be recalled at will, can it be modified?  For example, imagine a “tree” and, for most people, a tree comes to mind.  Meaning, they believe they can see a tree.  Of course the idea of seeing lies in the notion that the tree is in the mind.  The image of the tree is not like seeing the actual tree or even a picture of the tree.   Instead, the tree in the mind’s eye is what might be considered a rough copy of the tree.  The tree exists in the image, yet it is rough around the edges.  It takes practice in visualization to create the finer details that are more obvious with the person who has learned to create images.   The image of the tree is “somewhere” in the mind of the person imaging the tree.  To argue further that images do exist, picture a “car” but not just any car.  Picture a “Ford Explorer”.  Very likely, you either pictured a Ford Explorer you may have just recently seen or, you may have a Ford Explorer that is “white”.  If so, the “white, Ford Explorer” is probably the image you created.  Now, picture the same car but in “red”.  The mind is functional as is human thought.  Mental images exist regardless of the fact we cannot explain them or define them with specificity. 

What is important is that when a person visualizes, for example, a pine tree, the image is specific to that of a pine tree.  Now, to demonstrate the point somewhat further, when the same person is requested to visualize a huge mountain, the image is that of a huge mountain (taken from a stored memory of a mountain in a book, on TV or having climbed a mountain).  There really isn’t much difference even with sports.  Think about tennis, football, and gymnastics.  With the recall of each sport, the mind creates mental pictures consistent with known (stored) information specific to each sport.  This is not to say that language is not used to define specific thoughts or ideas, but to emphasize the likelihood that visual thinking is involved as well.  In fact, visual thinking can and does play an important role in recall or memory of information.  Once again, as an example, visualize a car (any car) and then visualize a truck.  In seconds, there is stored information that can be used to explain the differences between a car and a truck.

What is also interesting is that the image can be either two- or three-dimensional.  A person can visualize a truck from the driver’s side (or any side) and see only superior-inferior and left-to-right.  Or, a person can choose to see through the truck from any view and appreciate that it has depth (the 3rd dimension).  Either memory representation is useful in retrieving information, and the information itself is subject to the intentional representation (depending upon the information that is requested).  For example, if the intention is to visualize the shoulder flexors, the process of looking at and transforming images undergoes three steps: 

(1) Image generation (i.e., mentally picturing the shoulder flexors); 
(2) Image inspection (also called scanning the shoulder flexors); and 
(3) Image transformation (i.e., looking for the specific muscles that flex the shoulder joint). 
The process of looking at and transforming images encourages the retrieval of anatomical facts and important information about function and related muscles.  As an example, mentally picture the axillary nerve to the deltoid muscles.  It is located dorsal to the proximal end of the humerus.  By scanning the brachial plexus, it is possible to locate the axillary nerve arising from the posterior cord of the plexus just under the distal end of the clavicle.  If necessary, transform the image by either enlarging it or by rotating it.  Zooming in on the nerve is a simple matter of making the image larger.  Moving back from the image is the essence of making it smaller, perhaps to see all of the nerves that comprise the plexus.  Rotating the image also allows for information that otherwise might not be intuitively obvious. 

Consider your apartment or home in which you were raised as a young person.  Create an image of the physical structure as though you were standing at the front door.  Notice how essentially everything is part of the image (i.e., whether there are steps leading up to the front door, the type of door, the size of the door, the number of windows it may have, and how the windows are framed).  Now, allow your mind’s eye to simply rotate the image that represents the front door and the front of the dwelling.  Rotate the image of the physical structure that is to your left towards your right.  Notice that you can see the side of the dwelling that was previously to your left but out of site.  The rotation of the image that characterizes the dwelling is possible in every detail, in every direction, and is truly an image of multi-dimensions. 

To take the example further, allow yourself to become part of the image by visualizing yourself inside the living room of the dwelling.  Look straight ahead and think about what you see.  Your eyes may close or remain open.  Do you see your father’s favorite chair and/or desk?  What is the shape and size the chair?  Is it made of wood?  Is there a picture above the desk or a window?  How pieces of furniture are in the room?  How many doors lead from the room and to where?  Now, allow your mind to see the entire inside of the dwelling at once.  How many rooms are there?  How many bathrooms and bedrooms are there, and is the dwelling one or more floors?  Answers to these types of questions correspond essentially 100% with the actual structure and pieces of furniture throughout it.  What is interesting is that the human body and its various structures can be equally as obvious to the student and/or instructor who creates specific images that allow also for storage of important anatomical and/or physiological information. 

Instructors and/or students accustomed to visual thinking will also use the “blink” transformations in which the first image is allowed to fade (like the living room) and is replaced by a new, different image in its place (such as the dining room).  This technique is also used in athletics when a coach closes his/her eyes to see a new image of the skill at a new size, position, or location in the sequence of the skill.  Often the new image encourages the memory of information that might not be readily available with the first or even the second image.  Thus, the images that are created by the “blink transformation” process are useful in retrieving facts specific to a distinct issues and/or concerns.  Take the different parts of the right scapula as an example.  Visualize the dorsal view of the right scapula either positioned on the dorsal thorax of a person or at some distance in front of your eyes as a separate, stand alone bone.  Notice that the dorsal surface of the image approximates the dorsal view of the scapula if you were looking at it in an anatomy text.  Now, look at the dorsal spine, beginning at the base of the spine (which is located about one-third way down from the tip of the superior angle).  Follow the visual path of the spine laterally to the acromion process that is superior to the glenoid cavity.  Now, close your eyes and re-visualize the scapula by itself, three to four times its normal size, in front of your eyes and then open your eyes and image the scapula once again.  Look at the superior angle at the top left of the right scapula.  Contrast the osteological site with the inferior angle as the lowest tip of the scapula. Also, note that the two angles are created by borders.  The superior border of the scapula and the medial border create the superior angle.  The medial border and the lateral border create the inferior angle. 

In a very similar way, once again try this thought experiment.  Consider the door to your office or apartment (either if fine).  On which side is it hinged?  Does your office or apartments have windows?  How many windows does it have?  To answer these questions, you very likely, like most people, imagined your door and windows.  Now, image your desk in your office or your bed in the apartment.  Where is it located in the room?  What are the items positioned close to it or on top of it?  Answers to these questions are clearly a function of the images and the stored information that associates with the images in exactly the same way of understanding the various osteological sites of the scapula.  Therefore, it seems reasonable that, if the various structures positioned throughout your office or apartment are remembered by having previous knowledge of their size and position relative to each other, then the same storage of information is equally true with the osteological landmarks located on the scapula.  Information about the two (i.e., the office vs. the scapula) is derived from the images of each which then are subject to scanning and/or inspection in a manner analogous to the real objects.  The images function in the same way as having the experience of the real objects in hand and, if necessary, they are scanned, rotated, re-scaled, and otherwise mentally manipulated (transformed) to retrieve information.

The idea of “scanning” is especially interesting in accessing information from images.  As an example, aside from the visual and obvious differences in the humerus versus the femur, students can sequentially access information from less obvious sources (or images).  That is, by scanning the anterior surface of the humerus from the superior aspect to the inferior aspect, students should find that they experience an increase in time that is consistent with the reality of looking at a real humerus throughout its normal length.  In other words, scanning not only allows for specific information (such as the greater tubercle that is located on the proximal, anterior-lateral aspect of the humerus), it sets the stage for viewing the image as though the actual length of the humerus exists in the image itself. 

There is also a depth factor to consider when scanning the medial and lateral lips (borders) of the bicipital groove.  With the correct image, a person can sense the groove versus the elevated positions of the borders.  Of course, by moving the proximal end of the humerus forward to view the top of the bone, the groove is more pronounced with an obvious demonstration of the 3rd dimension (depth).  With another blink, the mind’s eye now locates the anterior distal aspect of the humerus and its relationship with the ulna (medial) and radius (lateral).  The respective medial and lateral epicondyles are easily visualized, thus allowing for additional anatomical information specific to points of origin of the anterior forearm flexors and the muscles that comprise the posterior compartment of the forearm, respectively. 

Vast amounts of information stored in memory can be accessed with all kinds of images.  However, the key to retrieving the stored information begins first with the storage process, which is usually in the form of memorization.  Specific information such as “the pectoralis major originates from the proximal anterior two-thirds of the clavicle, the full length of the sternum, and the costal cartilages of ribs one through five” begins with the memorization of the visual locations of each anatomical description.  Taken a step further, the pectoralis major inserts onto the lateral lip of the bicipital groove.  A picture before your mind’s eye allows for an understanding of the role of the muscle in medial rotation of the humerus, given that the vector is in the direction of the origin (as it is likely to be the more stable connection than the insertion).  Noting also the superior position of the muscle relative to its relatively lower and lateral insertion, the muscle can be visualized to produce shoulder flexion (particularly, the clavicular fibers).  Similarly, given the line of action for the sternal fibers, including the position of the vector versus the axis of the shoulder joint, the images portrays the lower fibers as having important influence on shoulder extension and adduction. 

By learning how to develop concepts (and of course the right answers) from pictures, students can be encouraged to develop an intellectual interest in the anatomy of motion and all types of athletic performance.  Visualizers have learned to appreciate the value in visualizing effects, consequences, and possibilities.  Students who are visualizers have an increased opportunity to learn and to appreciate the quality of movement that is rarely matched by students who think mainly in words (verbalizers).  Visualizers use images as a form of thinking.  Consider the following image of a person who is standing in front of you.  Look through the person’s clothing to see the chest.  Now, image the depth of the chest wall from the front of the person through the space where the lungs are found to the back of the thorax.  Close your eyes and open then again (using the blink transformation mentioned earlier).  See the same person but this time in layers of skin, fascia, muscle, and bone dimensions.  From the anterior view of the right side of the chest, image the origin of the serratus anterior muscle arising from the outer surfaces of ribs 1 through 9.  Notice the serrated (saw-tooth) appearance of the muscle as it takes origin from the anterior-lateral surfaces of the ribs.  With the person’s body standing directly in front of you, create an image that allows you to look through the thorax to visualize the anterior surface of the right scapula just behind the thoracic wall.  Specifically, look for the anterior surface of the medial border of the right scapula from the superior angle to the inferior angle.  This anatomical description is exactly the insertion of the serratus anterior muscle. 

Since muscles typically create a vector force that exerts an action(s) on a less stable bone, such as the scapula versus the rib cage, it should be relatively easy to visualize the movement of the right scapula towards the right side of the thoracic wall.  The movement is referred to as abduction (i.e., movement of the scapula away from the midline of the body, the spine).  However, since the primary purpose of the serratus anterior is to assist the shoulder flexors and/or abductors with upward rotation of the scapula, the insertion should be arranged to exert a force that pulls the inferior end of the scapula with a greater force than the force of abduction.  What is interesting is that, while all the points of origin exert an abducting force on the scapula, the points of origin arising specifically from ribs 4 through 9 exert an even more powerful abducting influence since they collectively insert predominately near the inferior angle of the scapula.  The image of the muscle from the anterior point of origin through the chest wall to the scapula allows for a better view of how the muscle assists the middle deltoid, arising from the acromion process, to position the scapula in upward rotation to thus allow for a greater range of motion when the deltoid (and the long head of the biceps brachii) contract to move the humerus away from the side of the body.

If teachers encourage the use of images in recalling anatomical facts (and even physiological facts), they encouraging the students to think.  When anyone is asked to think about an image, such as a bicycle versus a car, there are ideas and feelings that may have been forgotten but remembered when requested to “see” a bicycle or a car (and thus, the same with the deltoid muscle or the structure of the heart).  Hence, the use of images in kinesiology should help the students access information and to demonstrate control over important course material.  There is always the possibility that with individually realized images that new and previously unrealized information becomes evident.  This idea is not new.  Psychologists have studied images for many years.  They understand the importance of image formation and the interconnection with language, learning, and memory.  Perhaps, it is time that exercise physiologists consider the importance of imagery and how it can play an important role in cognitive function, athletic performance, and memory.

As a final example, consider that the teacher is interested in presenting the role of the cardiorespiratory system in the uptake and delivery of oxygen to the muscles.  The teacher could instruct the students to create an image of the lungs and their relationship, first, to the atmosphere and, second, the heart.  Naturally, as previously stated, a certain fundamental knowledge is important prior to and during the creation of the images to re-enforce terminology, relationships, and function.  From the air that surrounds the body, an inspired volume of about 500 ml fills the lungs with oxygen.  After adjustment that accounts for water vapor, visualize the lungs full of oxygen at a partial pressure of 100 mmHg in contrast to the oxygen in the mixed venous blood of 40 mmHg.  The pressure gradient of 60 mmHg creates movement of oxygen from the lungs into the pulmonary capillaries to associate with hemoglobin (Hb) to form oxyhemoglobin.  At the lung pressure of 100 mmHg and an average of 15 grams of Hb per 100 milliliters (ml) of blood, 20 ml of oxygen can be transported per 100 ml of blood. 

The heart pumps the blood saturated with oxygen to all the tissues of the body, especially to the muscles.  Imagine the flow of the blood from the lungs to the left atrium into the left ventricle.  During the contraction of the ventricles, called ventricular systole, the blood is placed under pressure which is ultimately the pressure that drives the blood from the heart into the vascular system.  The major vascular structures from the heart to the muscles of the left arm (as an example) include the images of the aorta (connected to the left ventricle), ascending aorta (the continuation of the major artery from the heart to the arteries of the head, upper body, and the upper limbs), brachiocephalic trunk, subclavian artery (the major arteries that contribute to the axillary artery in the armpit), and the brachial artery (the primary artery of the arm, the brachium) that divides into the radial and ulnar arteries. Close your eyes and visualize the arteries just outlined with a variety of smaller arteries arising from each that service related regional muscles.  Image the arteries full of oxygen making its way into the progressively smaller arterioles that have adjusted their size (lumen) by vasodilation to ensure that an ample volume of blood enters the capillaries.  The larger lumen size of the arterioles also allows for a reduction in the effort of the left ventricle, thus lowering what is referred to as systemic vascular resistance (or afterload). 

With the low resistance to the blood flow through the arteries and capillaries and the tissues’ need for oxygen, defined by a low partial pressure of 40 mmHg at rest and much lower during exercise, oxygen separates from Hb and moves from the capillaries to the individual muscle fibers.  Once oxygen is inside the muscle fibers, it is used to ensure that the energy development processes of the cells continue to match the tissues’ need for energy in the form of adenosine triphosphate (ATP).  Hence, oxygen becomes the final electron receptor in the electron transport system of the mitochondria.  The steps result in the formation of ATP, the energy currency of the muscle fiber, that fuels (allows) for muscle contraction.  The point here is that the images allow for the recall of facts and discourse.  It should be obvious that the steps in muscle contraction can easily be remembered with a little concerted effort to translate the excitation-coupling process as three or four connected images.  The time has come for exercise physiologists to take the knowledge that has revolutionized our field and integrate their thinking within the context of image.  It will transform the way students learn.

In summary, the myriad forms of transformation change occurring with images encourages the probing of each for the stored meaning and information that can be used to take tests, to organize a stress test protocol, or to lecture.  Images, when correctly perceived, are therefore, literally, the breakthrough point with remembering facts at a 3-dimensional level.  As Thoreau advised, "...if one advances confidently in the direction of his dreams, and endeavors to live the life which he has imagined, he will meet with a success unexpected in common hours."  It is logical that the same is true for success in learning exercise physiology.


Copyright ©1997-2001 American Society of Exercise Physiologists. All Rights Reserved.

ASEP Table of Contents
Questions/comments

Return to top of page