PEPonline
Professionalization
of Exercise Physiologyonline

An international electronic
journal for exercise physiologists
ISSN 1099-5862

Vol 4 No 3 March 2001

 

Using Criticial Thinking to Better 
Understand the Health Concerns of Passive Smoking
Tommy Boone, PhD, MPH, FASEP, EPC
Professor and Chair
Department of Exercise Physiology
The College of St. Scholastica
Duluth, MN


Introduction
The more one thinks about critical thinking, the more it fits as an absolute necessity of an education.  Students simply can’t profit from the illusion of thinking.  Professors, in particular, can do something about the illusion.  They have the physical space and the opportunity to bring order to the random connection of ideas.  Thus, the structuring of real thinking begins with an understanding of observations and possibilities. It is a matter of disconnecting from the tradition of beliefs for the hope of furthering the existence and power of the individual.  Critical thinking is therefore a freeing process; a step beyond the self-promoting ideas of yesterday or anyone with a reason to benefit.  Belief in something is powerful, but believing in the right “something” is the difference between being right and being a failure.

Teaching students about belief is as difficult as teaching students about the importance of critical thinking.  The wheels are already rolling; their thinking is already well captured.  How, then, can professors feel confident and/or right to alter the students’ course of events.  Frankly, the lack of a good answer to this question has probably kept some of my colleagues from trying.  The problem is that, even should they believe differently, the very act of not trying is often interpreted as a confirmation of what they are doing as being right.  Students have no reason to understand the difference, and thus the lack of disconfirming behavior masks the need for change.  Note that if the professors had pressed the issue of critical thinking, it is logical that the illusion of learning would have been recognized. 

Teaching is Just Teaching, Right?
In the grand scheme of things, “teaching is teaching, right”.  If so, then anyone can be a teacher and, frankly, it just isn’t so!  Even among teachers, those with the PhD, there are many who can’t teach and many who care less about teaching.  Merely having the PhD, as a criterion for excellence in teaching, is as senseless as having a key to a car means that the person can drive.  We expect more from professors and, especially the PhD prepared exercise physiologist.  All else being equal, the PhD should mean something.  And, it should mean more than a ticket to a college position or to validate the professor.  It is easy to get the big head, but it is much harder to split time with what should be done on behalf of students and the profession and what is typically done to benefit the professor.  Increasingly, this difference is becoming the target of distinction and thus the difference between the extraordinary performance and the average performance.  But, my professer is different, I've heard.  "I am happy about what I'm learning at this college.  What you’re saying doesn’t apply to my situation.”  What these statements have in common is that they are often cited in support of a person’s beliefs.  Students are convinced that the program offerings are complete and justified.  Such convictions are good, but only if the evidence supports the beliefs.  As an example, what if the distinction between the student’s academics at one institution (say, exercise science) is little more than “one” course from that of another student in a different institution with an entirely different degree (e.g., physical education)?  Indeed, if only one course separates the exercise science concentration from the physical education degree, the degree is then a physical education degree. 

The most likely reason for the lack of confusion, that is, the lack of understanding of the differences between exercise science and physical education is that the PhD exercise physiologists are glad to have a college position.  Why should they tell the students?  The layout, according to the past 50 years or so, is obviously beneficial to traditional physical educators.  Whether similar parallels exist in academia is an interesting question.  In view of what continues to happen within the infrastructure of hundreds of university settings, few college teachers with degrees in kinesiology, exercise science, physical education, and/or exercise physiology appear interested in lending credibility to a “pure” exercise physiology program.  Similarly, the uninformed remains convinced of what they are doing is right or they are afraid to experiment with new ideas and/or possibilities.  The problem is that disconfirmation begins with critical thinking and, unless the PhD professors have been taught how to think versus what to think, the likelihood of confirming new ideas is very small.  The end result is more professors who are concerned with wanting to keep things as they are than with considering hidden possibilities in change.

The important point here is that we cannot forget the importance of teaching critical thinking.  Those who do so understand the importance of thinking straight.  Those who don’t often have to address the erroneous conclusions of unclear thinking.  If only our institutions were filled with critical thinkers, the hazards of uncritical thinking would not be underestimated. This idea is illustrated even more clearly by considering the relationship between low-density lipoprotein cholesterol and coronary artery disease (CAD).  If there is a strong relationship, then the obvious strategy would be to lower cholesterol to lower the probability of suffering from CAD.  Left unchecked, high cholesterol causes CAD, right?  Unfortunately, on behalf of mankind, the answer isn’t all that simple.  There are many factors that appear to associate with CAD and, therefore, angina pectoris or a myocardial infarction (MI).  As devastating as it is to have an MI, it is incorrect to believe that it is caused solely by  high cholesterol.  There are simply too many “risk” factors for CAD.  Very likely, the loss of myocardial integrity is a direct function of genetics; a scientific point of view that is incalculable and will remain to be for decades.

The Problem of Hidden Data
Moving away from the traditional way of thinking requires a distinct discrimination.  It is all about the problem of hidden or absent data that influences the kinds of inferences professors (and students) draw and, therefore, their beliefs about exercise and heart disease.  Often times, students are presented the obvious views and the obvious relationships between disease and lifestyle issues instead of the more complex and, frankly, the more mentally and emotionally gratifying understanding of assumed beliefs.

Unfortunately, with the increased need to find “the” cause for different diseases, there is an unfortunate (and unscientific) movement to think very narrowly.  The resulting problem is one of a self-fulfilling prophecy.  That is, although cholesterol may be associated with CAD, the association as such doesn’t establish a “cause and effect” relationship.  This particular view, however outside of mainstream thinking, is important to fully understanding the various multi-faceted associations with CAD.  Obviously, the exact cause or causes remain to be determined.

This is but one example of hidden thinking; the teaching of ideas without full disclosure of all the data.  There are many examples of uncritical thinking that result in the victimization of the public.  It is critical that the professor doesn’t neglect the full set of data when lecturing on the relationship of one or more variables to a disease and/or disorder.  Making all the data available to the student helps to prevent mis-understandings and the belief that should the disease surface, “I did it to myself, there isn’t anyone else to blame but me.”  How can the student and/or individual in the community, with similar coaching, not believe the self-fulfilling prophecy?  Hence, the absence of critical thinking and “critical lecturing” sets the emotional circumstances for a premature understanding of the disease. 

Beliefs are Difficult to Give Up
The tendency to make fast judgments by exercise physiologists who have an obvious interest in the cardiovascular system is “the” condition of today’s teaching.  It is likely to be full of errors and an over-reliance on the simplification of disease agents.  Professors are not deliberately driven to confuse or hide behind a common belief.  Instead, they believe certain things because they were taught “X” causes “Y”.   Beliefs are difficult to give up, especially if they are associated with decades of thinking.  Only by taking responsibility to think clearly and objectively is it possible to dispel commonly accepted views.  As an example, take the assumed relationship between health and negative mental imagery.  Is it true that a person’s health is susceptible to negative thinking?  What if it isn’t true?  Have their been too many published books on mind-body issues, stress management lectures, and psycho-self-help workshops?  The short answer is that it is likely that we have reached beyond the reality of the data.  Our hope in avoiding a specific disease is not consistent with the unknown causes and occurrence of disease even in the most dedicated believer.  It is also possible that some individuals, including professionals in the healthcare field, would not want to go through life thinking that their emotions dictate the type of disease they may suffer from at some point in life. 

Indeed, the idea that our mental health conforms to our mental images has always been problematic on several fronts.  One, it is very likely the general belief that thinking right will keep the body’s physiology right is unfounded.  Personally, I understand that the human body isn’t so predictable, given its multi-complexity and mystery.  There is also the problem that if good thoughts and mental pictures were to result in good health, then everyone who gets sick is subject to blame him- or herself.  Blaming the victim is too easy and should be rejected.  Physically disabled individuals either with cancer or a chronic inflammatory disease should not have to live under the false impression that they are responsible for their own misfortune. 

While our knowledge of the power of the mind in preventing or curing diseases is too uncertain to rely completely on mental images, it is not illogical to understand how negative thoughts can be self-defeating.  If we are to succeed, we must make an effort and, yes, we must believe in ourselves.  There is a difference between thinking positive to help us get moving in the right direction and “mental phenomenon” that is scientifically without a guarantee of sound science.   At first glance, it could be argued that “the truth” will be known.  Meaning, surely, cancer is caused by a bad attitude.  College students and the general public are told that the mind influences the body.  Perhaps it does, but is influence the same as causing cancer?  The lesson that is important is the answer to the question, “What does the evidence really show?”  It may be best to begin an examination of whether the evidence exists by using the skeptics approach to the problem of passive smoking.  Have scientists misplaced data, tampered with the data, or made conclusions without considering all the facts.  If so, the net effect may be the validation of the researchers’ views. 

How would the reader know that the paper he/she is reading is a well-written product marketed for publication?  Most would not know that published papers are a collection of calculated assumptions to astonish the reviewers in hopes of being acceptable for publication.  Because publishing is so important for tenure, promotion, and financial gain, it is unfortunately the case that some professors write either knowingly or unknowingly in a manner that confirms their bias.  The challenge of course is to write as objectively as possible and, thus avoid the errors and biases of human judgment.  The following information about passive smoking is presented to encourage the reader to take a step back and ask, “Does the information put closure on the belief that passive smoking causes a variety of healthcare problems or is there sufficient doubt to conclude that passive smoking is not the cause of certain pathologies?”  

Passive Smoking: Is it Dangerous?
While the mainstream smoke inhaled into the larger airways of the lungs leaves particles that are considered harmful [1], the involuntary exposure to the smoke that circulates into the air between puffs may also be harmful and, perhaps, deadly to non-smokers [2].  This smoke is called sidestream smoke, and presently there is a major debate in the scientific literature as to whether it is harmful [3,4].  This paper identifies the medical and research issues in the debate on passive smoke and lung cancer.

The Concerns
The dangers associated with passive smoke have been debated for more than 20 years.  However, only recently has the exposure to sidestream smoke, as a health concern, been identified and concluded as a major cause of indoor air pollution.  In fact, according to the Environmental Protection Agency, passive smoking is classified as a class A carcinogenic environmental condition with more than 40 carcinogenic compounds.  The conclusion, if there is no safe threshold for carcinogens, is that sidestream smoke carries with it sufficient reason to question the exposure [5],

Unfortunately, the non-smoker is generally unaware of the contents of sidestream smoke, particularly N-nitrosamine, benzo-[a]-pyrene, carbon monoxide, and heavy metals that may cause cancer [6,7].  The non-smoker may also be poorly informed about various irritations induced by sidestream smoke, particularly symptoms from irritation of the eyes and throat to headache and coughing.  The otherwise healthy non-smoker who is allergic to cigarette smoke may experience wheezing, sneezing, and nausea [8,9].  The unhealthy person with coronary artery disease may experience increased episodes of chest pain (angina pectoris) and/or electrical irregularities of the myocardium when exposed to passive smoke [10].

Aside from passive smoke being a source of irritation and a nuisance, it may increase the risk of lung cancer.  The results of numerous studies have supported this notion [11-14].  However, these studies have been criticized for the small number of subjects with differing designs, for subject participation from different hospitals, and for lack of histological confirmation [14].  As to the latter concern, the 1983 study by Correa et al. [15], however, demonstrated histological confirmation for 97% of the cases.  Their work, that agrees with Trichopolous et al. [11]. strengthens the suspicion that passive smoke may contribute to lung cancer.  Both studies support earlier contention [12] that heavy smoking by one member of the spouse pair increases the lung cancer risk of the non-smoking partner.

Most of the cases of lung cancer in the non-smoking partner are found in women [16,17].  The reason for the female's increased incidence of lung cancer when exposed to secondhand smoke is unclear.  First, sidestream cigarette smoke, although containing toxic components [18], is diluted in the ambient air and, second, the bronchial epithelial changes in the smoker are rarely found in the non-smoker [19].  This latter point is important since results associated with passive smoke must differ with reports of different types of lung cancer [20].  For example, Trichopoulos et al. [11] reported a histologically confirmed diagnoses of 35%; whereas, Chan and Fung [21] reported 82%.  Some studies [14] excluded adenocarcinoma while it predominated in others [12,20,21]. 

There is also the question of previous studies focusing just on the spouse's smoking.  Kabat and Wynder [20] pointed out that the measure of exposure to passive smoke is influenced by the time the non-smoking spouse is in the presence of the smoker and whether the smoker is a light or heavy smoker.  There is also the additive factor of previous exposure to smoke by earlier marriages, relatives, and at restaurants and at work, or by travel via trains and buses.

The Mechanisms
The question remains, "By what mechanism does passive smoke cause lung cancer?"  Theoretically, the volatile components of cigarette smoke cause lesions [22].  Nitrosamine content, in particular, in passive smoke is reported by Stock [23] to be some 50 times greater than in mainstream smoke.  Other reports [24,25] indicate that sidestream smoke has 52 times as much dimethylnitrosamine, 16 times as much naphthalene, 28 times as much methylnophtholene, 3.4 times as much benzo-[a]-pyrene, and 5.6 times as much toluene as mainstream smoke. 

Naturally, the actual exposure to these chemicals depends on factors mentioned earlier [20] plus the volume of smoke generated, the volume of air, room ventilation, and the differences in which the cigarette is smoked.  There are also other confounding influences.  Auerback and Garfinkel [26] stated that radiation treatment (with exposure to ionizing radiation) may lead to lung cancer.  There is also the possibility of lung cancer developing in the non-smoker subsequent to healed tuberculosis scars [27] or, in the female non-smoker, as an estrogen-related tumor (adenocarcinoma) [26].

The reports of mutagens in the urine of passive smokers [29] suggest the possibility of increased cancer risk for specific non-respiratory sites.  This point is supported by the data from Sandler et al. [30] who found a significant association between passive smoke and lung cancer [odds ratio (OR) of 1.9].  They reported OR for other sites (such as cancer of the cervix) that were statistically significant. In agreement, Buckley et al. [31] found a 4-fold risk of cervical cancer among passive smokers.  However, following statistical adjustment for the husbands' number of sexual partners, the risk decreased to 2-fold that was not significant.  Although similar findings have been reported by Brown and associates [32]. Hirayama [12] reported that the risk for cervical cancer was not increased. 

Wald et al. [7] reported a 35% increase in the risk of lung cancer among non-smokers living with smokers compared with non-smokers living with non-smokers.  They concluded that about a third of the cases of lung cancer in non-smokers who lived with smokers may be attributed to passive smoke.  This view is supported by the earlier study by Wald and Ritchie [33] in which the average urinary cotinine concentration among non-smokers married to smokers was about three times that among non-smokers married to non-smokers.

If urinary cotinine levels reflect exposure to carcinogens in passive smoke, then the findings of increased lung cancer risk among non-smokers might be expected.  Cotinine levels among non-smokers living with 2-pack-a-day smokers have been reported to be roughly the same in smokers of less than 3 cigarettes per day [34].   If there is a relationship between urinary cotinine levels, carcinogens, and passive smoke, it is not an easy one to duplicate.  Hence, even though researchers have concluded that passive smoke may increase the non-smokers' risk of lung cancer, they have stated as well that not all studies have shown significant associations [20,35].  Moveover, even with the pooling of data on non-smokers from three case-control studies, Dalager et al. [36] concluded that the small number of cases precluded a definitive answer on the carcinogenic effects of passive smoke.

The Problems
The problem with the statement that passive smoking causes lung cancer is the omission of other factors as risk factors for lung cancer.  Take, for example, the study by Alvanja and associates [37] in which the authors found a significant 2.2-fold age-adjusted risk of lung cancer in non-smoking women who had not smoked for at least 15 years compared with lifetime non-smokers.  Here is an indication that non-smokers are subject to lung cancer, but rather than passive smoke as the cause -- it was the residual effects of having smoked earlier in one's lifetime! 

Alvanga et al. [37] also reported that 41% of the women with lung cancer reported having at least one previous lung disease.  They found that the risk of lung cancer following previous lung disease was similar among lifetime non-smokers (OR = 1.4) and former smokers (OR = 1.5).  Asthma and pneumonia were significantly related to lung cancer (OR = 2.7 and 1.5, respectively) among lifetime non-smokers, and that previous lung disorders were significantly related to both adenocarcinomas (OR = 1.4) and all other cell types combined (OR = 1.8).

The mechanism by which pre-existing lung diseases may increase the risk of lung cancer in lifelong non-smokers is not known.  Asthma may increase the time within the lung for carcinogenic exposure to contact the epithelial cells [37].  Pneumonia may increase lung cancer risk (adenocarcinomas, in particular) if it results in lung scars [26].  These findings support the contention of some researchers [38] that chronic obstructive pulmonary disease may be associated with increased risk of lung cancer.

What is strikingly important in the work by Alvanja et al. [37] is that they concluded approximately 13% of the lung cancer among lifelong non-smokers is attributable to previous lung disease!  This point is not considered in most studies.  Also, many articles fail to adequately address the role domestic radon exposure, diet, occupation, and familiar factors may account for lung cancer among non-smokers.

Instead, the growing awareness that passive smoke is said to cause lung cancer [5,39-42] is witnessed by the increased restrictions on where people can smoke [43].  In other words, according to some experts, the opinion is that biological plausibility of the epidemiological findings has been established and, thus causality has been established as well.  Consider, for example, the conclusion by Glantz and Parmley [3] that "...passive smoking causes...lung cancer...."  However, simply concluding the issue in a scientific manner via various scientific journals does not mean that passive smoke causes lung cancer.  Equally true is the notion that just because some people find active and/or passive smoking "genuinely distasteful" [44-46] does not make either type of smoking "the" cause of lung cancer.

Overall, however, it is hard to overlook the role of various mutagenic substances in cigarette smoke, especially since greater than 80% of all cases of lung cancer in the United States is believed to be due to cigarette smoke [47].  Nitrosamine and polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons may contribute to an early carcinogenic process [37], particularly with regard to mutations in the p53 tumor suppressor gene [48].  Other contributors may be the role various promotional exposures to phenols, diet, and numerous other lifestyle factors in the development of lung cancer.  The likelihood of individuals with inherited predispositions to smoking-related carcinomas cannot be discounted either, and may become a major issue in the future with new research in molecular genetics [47].

Until then, there is still the recognized association between cigarette smoke and renal cancer [49], myeloid leukemia [50], and cancer of the pancreas [51] to mention a few.  These observations are consistent with the magnitude of the risk of lung cancer from passive smoke.  But, given these findings, one would expect that the particulates, which are reported to be 10 times greater in mainstream smoke than in passive smoke, would result in even more lung cancer than cited in the literature.  Moreover, in that the cigarette smoker inhales about 420 mg of tar per day (14 mg per cigarette) versus the non-smoker's exposure to 1.43 mg of tar per day [52], active smoking should be significantly more damaging to the lungs with a greater predisposition to significantly more lung cancer but is it? 

Lung cancer appears to be associated with a multitude of factors in some subjects while relative few in others.  The debate whether cigarette smoke causes lung cancer is confounded since lung cancer is associated with numerous conditions.  Consider, for example, in Utah where the per capita cigarette consumption was 1,500 in 1976, the death rate from lung cancer among males for 1950 through 1969 was 22/100,000 -- the lowest in the United States according to Rawson [53].  The author stated that in Idaho, where the per capita cigarette consumption was 2,500, (1,000 more than in Utah), the male death rate from lung cancer was 23/100,000, and in Wyoming, where the male death rate from lung cancer was 27/100,000, the 1976 per capita consumption of cigarettes was 3,200 or twice that of Utah.  Colorado had a 1976 per capita cigarette consumption of 2,700 and a male death rate from lung cancer of 28/100,000.  Rawson [53] stated that the relatively low lung cancer death rates in states adjacent to Utah are not too different from the low death rate among Utahans in spite of significantly greater cigarette consumption rates.

In addition, although urinary cotinine is reported to be correlated with exposure to passive smoke, there is the question of biologic significance of the exposure.  Wald and colleagues [54] reported median urinary cotinine levels of 1,655 ng/ml in cigarette smokers, 6 ng/ml in passive smokers, and about 2 ng/ml in non-smokers not exposed to passive smoke.  Given these values, it is difficult to understand that if smokers with reported urinary cotinine levels as high as 1,655 versus 6 ng/ml (and passive smoking is said to cause lung cancer), then all smokers should have lung cancer.  Yet the reality of the research findings is such that it is impossible to conclude that all smokers will develop lung cancer.  About all one can say is that  "...passive smoking may result in an increased morbidity and mortality among non-smokers" [55].  The link between passive smoke and lung cancer is not as straightforward as frequently reported [56].

The Questions
An interesting question is "Why the fuss over lung cancer from passive smoke?"  When, according to Wells [57] and Kawachi et al. [58], the number of heart disease deaths due to passive smoke is an order of magnitude greater (37,000) than the number of lung cancer deaths due to passive smoke (3,700).  The problem with passive smoke would appear to be heart disease deaths not lung cancer deaths!  Deaths from other cancers (12,000) increase the total to 15,700, which is still less than one-half of the deaths due to heart disease even though the relative risks for heart disease and lung cancer in association with passive smoke were similar, that is, 1.3 for both diseases [59,60].

Regardless of the impression one may get from various news sources and scientific journals, there is also the question of whether it is right and, if so, to what degree to regulate the presence of cigarette smoke in the workplace and to ban cigarettes entirely if it seems warranted?  There is the very real possibility that despite the regulatory measures, the rate of lung cancer will be maintained since there are so many carcinogens affecting the general population.  In fact, it is highly probable that no one potent carcinogen is the culprit.

The likelihood of significant synergistic effects of numerous carcinogens should be considered when "the" cause, such as passive smoke, is discussed in association with a specific cancer (such as lung cancer).  Who knows how many of the non-smoking wives of the published studies were at some point exposed to asbestos?  Who has studied the role high attitudes (such as Denver) and flying at high altitudes may have on the incidence of lung cancer from cosmic radiation?  Have the authors of the various studies considered the geographic influence on their subjects' cancer?   Do immunosuppressive drugs contribute to an increased risk of lung cancer?

In men with Klinefelter's syndrome, the risk of developing breast cancer is 66 times that in normal men [61].  Is it possible that lung cancer, for example, whether in the presence of passive smoke or not is associated with abnormal chromosomes?  Is a certain genetic factor associated with the eventual development of lung cancer?  Do susceptible individuals in affected families develop the same type of cancer [62], and have the epidemiologists carefully examined this factor in their research?  Very likely only a small proportion of the individuals exposed to passive smoke will manifest lung cancer.  These individuals probably have a constitutional predisposition that increases their individual susceptibility to the cigarette smoke.

The problem of latency, that is, the period between first exposure and onset of disease may vary between 5 and 50 years, [63] has not been thoroughly investigated.  Is it possible that passive smoke may serve to influence the latency period of lung cancer that may have been initiated by some other known or unknown occupational agent?  If the answer is "maybe" or "yes" then, if researchers fail to address the type, strength, and interaction of exposure to other chemicals, their findings may be correctly interpreted as premature and/or misleading. 

Unfortunately, given the complexity of the multi-dimensional nature of risk factors affecting human beings, crucial retrospective dose data are often not available.  Also, diagnostic criteria and methods not only change over time but may be interpreted differently as well.  The subject's perceived exposure or lack of exposure to a particular chemical may be subject to a variety of biased views.  The task of identifying and documenting past exposures is a researcher's nightmare, which is often incomplete without careful consideration of what it means to the outcome of the study.

Aside from these concerns, the official position of the National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health is "...exposure to any known or suspected carcinogen must be reduced to the lowest level possible by whatever means available" [64-65].  Obviously, one way to do this, regarding passive smoke, is to eliminate active smoke.  This decision would seem appropriate since it is difficult to determine the "safe" level of human exposure to passive smoke with any degree of confidence.  Some authorities [64] would still argue that passive smoke has not been proven to be a carcinogen. 

Summary
Experimental epidemiological studies face many problems, and whether they are retrospective or prospective studies does not matter that much.  What matters is the accuracy of the measurement of all possible confounding influences.  Because it is practically impossible to measure everything, the studies are at best crude estimates of associations that may or may not be statistically and/or biologically significant.

What would be refreshing in many of these reports is the authors' attempt at being fair and reasonable with their conclusions.  Too often the only hint of their trustworthiness, as scientific thinkers, is the relatively frequent use of the word "may."  By concluding that passive smoke may or seems to cause lung cancer, the researchers leave the door open to the reader that the published manuscript attempts to allow for both sides of a controversial topic.  But, is this approach and its apparent inadequate covering of alternative views of the same topic a fair and critical (straight thinking) analysis?  Probably not!  The truth about lung cancer and passive smoke, as defined by epidemiological studies, is that the findings apply to whole populations and not individuals.  Dickman [66] points out that, "Although the conclusion may be interesting scientifically, it has little practical value."  Hence, unfortunately, risk factors cannot be used to predict which individual will get lung cancer from passive smoke if in fact passive smoke causes lung cancer.

Is it possible that much of the literature on passive smoke and lung cancer has made the typical individual on the street less concerned about lung cancer if he/she does not smoke or does not associate with smokers?  The answer is "yes" but does this individual have all the facts?  The answer is "no" and, therefore, by not educating the students (and the public at large) to take full responsibility for health matters, including the interpretation of research findings, both have become more comfortable in certainty than with probabilities. The probability is, regardless of individual similarities, individuals differ in ways little understood by most researchers.  The difficulty in extrapolating from rodents to humans is easy enough to understand, but there is also the problem (although more difficult to understand by most researchers) of extrapolation from many humans to just one human.


References
1. Weiss ST, Tager IB, Schenker M, et al. The health effects of involuntary smoking. Am Rev Respir Dis 1983; 128: 933-942.
2. Lesmes GR, Donofrio KH. Passive Smoking: The medical and economic issues. Am J Med 1992; 93: 1A-38S-1A-42S.
3. Glantz SA, Parmley WW. Passive smoking causes heart disease and lung cancer. J Clin Epidemiol 1992; 45: 815-819.
4. Mantel N. Dubious evidence of heart and cancer deaths due to passive smoking. J Clin Epidemiol 1992; 45: 809-813.
5. Health effects of passive smoking: assessment of lung cancer in adults and respiratory disorders in children. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Environmental Protection Agency, Office of Health and Environmental Assessment, Office of Atmospheric and Indoor Air Programs, May 1990. External Review Draft, Report No. EPA-600-6-90-006A.
6. The health consequences of smoking - cancer. Washington, D.C.: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, 1982. Report No. PHS 82-50179.
7. Wald NJ, Nanchahal K, Thompson SG, et al. Does breathing other people's tobacco smoke cause lung cancer? Br Med J (Clin Res Ed) 1986: 293: 1217-1222.
8. Speer F. Tobacco and the nonsmoker. A study of subjective symptoms. Arch Environ Health 1968; 16: 443-446.
9. Weber A, Jermini C, Grandjean E. Irritation effects on man of air pollution due to cigarette smoke. Am J Public Health 1976; 66: 672-676.
10. Aronow WS. Effect of passive smoking on angina pectoris. N Engl J Med 1978; 299: 21-24.
11. Trichopoulos D, Kalandidi A, Sparros L, et al. Lung cancer and passive smoking. Int J Cancer 1981; 27: 1-4.
12. Hirayama T. Non-smoking wives of heavy smokers have a higher risk of lung cancer: A study from Japan. Br Med J 1981; 282:183-185.
13. Garfinkel L. Time trends in lung cancer mortality among non-smokers and a note on passive smoking. J Nat Cancer Inst 1981; 66: 1061-1066.
14. Trichopoulos D, Kalandidi A, Sparros L. Lung cancer and passive smoking: Conclusion of Greek study. Lancet 1983; ii: 677-678.
15. Correa P, Pickle LW, Fontham E, et al. Passive smoking and lung cancer. Lancet 1983; ii: 595-597.
16. Wynder EL. Tobacco as a cause of lung cancer, with special reference to the infrequency of lung cancer among non-smokers. Pennsylvania Med J 1954; 57: 1073-1083.
17. Wynder EL, Berg JW. Cancer of the lung among nonsmokers: Special reference to histologic patterns. Cancer 1967; 20: 1161-1172.
18. Brunnemann KD, Adams JD, Ho DPS, et al. The influence of tobacco smoke on indoor atmospheres: II. Volatile and tobacco-specific nitrosamine in main and sidestream smoke and their contribution to indoor air pollution. Proceedings of the Fourth Joint Conference on Sensing of Environmental Pollutants, New Orleans, Louisiana. 1978: 876-880.
19. Auerbach O, Garfinkel L, Hammond EC. Changes in bronchial epithelium in relation to cigarette smoking. 1955 - 1969 versus 1970 - 1977. N Engl J Med 1979; 300: 381-386.
20. Kabat GC, Wynder EL. Lung cancer in nonsmokers. Cancer 1984; 53: 1214-1221
21. Chan WC, Fung SC. Lung cancer in non-smokers in Hong Kong. In Grundmann E. (Ed). Cancer Campaign, Vol 6. Cancer Epidemiology. Stuttgart, New York: Gustav Fischer Verlag, 1982: 199-202.
22. Aronow WS. Effects of passive smoking on angina pectoris. N Engl J Med 1978; 229: 21-24.
23. Stock SL. Risks the passive smoker runs. Lancet 1980; ii: 1082.
24. Smoking and health: A report of the Surgeon General. Department of Health and Human Services. Publication No. (PHS); 1979: 79-50066.
25. The health consequences of smoking: The changing cigarette. Department of Health and Human Services. Publication No. (PHS); 1981: 50156.
26. Auerbach O, Garfinkel L, Parks VR. Scar cancer of the lung: Increase over a 21-year-period. Cancer 1979; 43: 636-642.
27. Hinds MW, Cohen HI, Kolonel LN. Tuberculosis and lung cancer risk in nonsmoking women. Am Rev Respir Dis 1982; 125: 776-778.
28. Chaudhuri PK, Thomas PA, Walker MJ, et al. Steroid receptors in human lung cancer cytosols. Cancer Letters 1982; 16: 327-332.
29. Manchester DK, Jacoby EH. Sensitivity of human placental monooxygenase activity to maternal smoking. Clin Pharmacol Ther 1981; 30: 687-692.
30. Sandler DP, Everson RB, Wilcox AJ. Passive smoking in adulthood and cancer risk. Am J Epidemiol 1985; 121: 37-48.
31. Buckley JD, Harris RWC, Doll R, et al. Case-control study of the husbands of women with dysplasia or carcinoma of the cervix uteri. Lancet 1981; 2: 1010-1015.
32. Brown DC, Pereira L, Garner JB. Cancer of the cervix and the smoking husband. Can Fam Physician 1982; 28: 499-502.
33. Wald NJ, Ritchie C. Validation of studies on lung cancer in non-smokers married to smokers. Lancet 1984: i: 1067.
34. Akiba S, Kato H, Blot WJ. Passive smoking and lung cancer among Japanese women. Cancer Res 1986; 46: 4804-4807.
35. Koo LC, Ho JH, Saw D. Is passive smoking an added risk factor for lung cancer in Chinese women. J Exp Clin Cancer Res 1984; 3: 277-283.
36. Dalager NA, Pickle LW, Mason TJ, et al. The relation of passive smoking to lung cancer. Cancer Res 1986; 46: 4808-4811.
37. Alvanja MCR, Brownson RC, Boice JD, et al. Preexisting lung disease and lung cancer among nonsmoking women. Am J Epidemiol 1992; 136: 623-632.
38. Skillrud DM, Offord KP, Miller RD. Higher risk of lung cancer in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease: A prospective matched controlled study. Ann Intern Med 1986; 105: 503-507.
39. United States Surgeon General. The health consequences of involuntary smoking: A report of the Surgeon General. Rockville, MD: U.S. Public Health Service, Department of Health and Human Services; 1986.
40. National Research Council. Environmental tobacco smoke: Measuring exposure and assessing health effects. Washington, DC: National Academy Press; 1986.
41. United States Department of Health and Human Services. Environmental tobacco smoke in the workplace: Lung cancer and other health effects. NIOSH Current Intelligence Bulletin 54; June; 1991.
42. World Health Organization, International Agency for Research on Cancer. IARC monographs on the evaluation of the carcinogenic risk of chemicals to humans. Vol 38, Tobacco smoking. Lyon, France: IRAC; 1986.
43. United States Surgeon General. Reducing the health consequences of smoking - 25 years of progress: A report of the Surgeon General. Rockville, MD: U.S. Public Health Service, Department of Health and Human Services; 1989.
44. On the move: Environmental tobacco smoke and public transport. London: Tobacco Advisory Council; 1990.
45. Nine to five: Environmental tobacco smoke and the workplace. London: Tobacco Advisory Council; 1990.
46. Out and about: Environmental tobacco smoke and leisure. London: Tobacco Advisory Council; 1990.
47. Carbone D. Smoking and Cancer. Am J Med 1992; 93: 135-139.
48. Harris CC. Chemical and physical carcinogenesis: Advances and perspectives for the 1990s. Cancer Res 1992; 51: 5023-5044.
49. Mclaughlin JK, Hrubec Z, Heineman EF, et al. Renal cancer and cigarette smoking in a 26-year followup of U.S. veterans. Public Health Report 1990; 105: 535.
50. Mills PK, Newell GR, Beeson WL, et al. History of cigarette smoking and risk of leukemia and myeloma: Results from the Adventist health study. J Natl Cancer Inst 1990; 82: 1832-1836.
51. Howe GR, Jain M, Burch JD, et al. Cigarette smoking and cancer of the pancreas: Evidence from a population-based case-control study in Toronto, Canada. Int J Cancer 1991; 47: 323-328.
52. Repace JL, Lowrey AH. A quantitative estimate of nonsmokers' lung cancer risk from passive smoking. Environ Int 1985; 11: 3-22.
53. Rawson RW. The epidemiology of health - A new frontier toward the prevention of cancer. In HB Demopoulos and MA Mehlman. Cancer and the environment. Park Forest South, Illinois: Pathotox Publishers, Inc., 1980: 103-112.
54. Wald NJ, Boreham J, Bailey A, et al. Urinary cotinine as marker of breathing other people's tobacco smoke. Lancet 1984; 1: 230-231.
55. Svendsen KH, Kuller LH, Martin MJ, et al. Effects of passive smoking in the multiple risk factor intervention trial. Am J Epidemiol 1987; 126: 783-795.
56. Doll R, Hill AB. A study of the aetiology of carcinoma of the lung. Br Med J 1952; 2: 1271-1286.
57. Wells A. An estimate of adult mortality in the United States from passive smoking. Environ Int 1988; 14: 249-265.
58. Kawachi I, Pearce N, Jackson R. Deaths from lung cancer and ischaemic heart disease due to passive smoking in New Zealand. N Z Med J 1989; 102: 337-340.
59. Wells A. An estimate of adult mortality in the United States from passive smoking: A response to criticism. Environ Int 1990; 16: 187-193.
60. Glantz SA, Parmley WW. Passive smoking and heart disease: Epidemiology, physiology, and biochemistry. Circulation 1991; 83: 1-12.
61. Mulvihill JJ. Congenital and genetic diseases. In Fraumeni JF (Ed.). Persons at high risk of cancer: An approach to etiology and control. New York, NY: Academic Press, 1975: 3-37.
62. Anderson DE. Familial susceptibility. In Fraumeni JF (Ed.). Persons at high risk of cancer: An approach to etiology and control. New York, NY: Academic Press, 1975: 39-54.
63. Schottenfeld D, Hass JF. Carcinogens in the workplace. In Sax NI. Cancer causing chemicals. New York, NY: Van Nostrand Reinhold Company, 1981: 14-27.
64. Fairchild EJ, II. Guidelines for a NIOSH policy on occupational carcinogenesis. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1976; 271: 200-207.
65. Estimates of the fraction of cancer in the United States related to occupational factors. Report prepared by the National Cancer Institute, the National Institute of Environmental Health Sciences and National Institute for Occupational Safety and Health. September 15; 1978.
66. Dickman SR. Pathways to wellness. Champain, Illinois: Life Enhancement Publications; 1988. 


Copyright ©1997-2001 American Society of Exercise Physiologists. All Rights Reserved.

ASEP Table of Contents
Questions/comments

Return to top of page